Industries in Depth

Why do we end up in echo chambers? And can we get out?

People look at their mobile phones while waiting for a train at a subway station in Tokyo, Japan, October 14, 2015.  Japanese manufacturers' confidence worsened for the second straight month and is expected to fade going forward, a Reuters poll showed, adding to lingering fears of a recession and keeping policymakers under pressure to deploy fresh stimulus. Picture taken October 14, 2015. REUTERS/Yuya Shino

Who should we blame – ourselves, or filtering algorithms beyond our control? Image: REUTERS/Yuya Shino

Sagit Bar-Gill
Postdoctoral associate, MIT Sloan School of Management
Neil Gandal
Professor of Economics, Berglas School of Economics Tel Aviv University
Share:
Our Impact
What's the World Economic Forum doing to accelerate action on Industries in Depth?
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Media, Entertainment and Sport is affecting economies, industries and global issues
A hand holding a looking glass by a lake
Crowdsource Innovation
Get involved with our crowdsourced digital platform to deliver impact at scale
Stay up to date:

Media, Entertainment and Information

Just as sound reverberates in an enclosed space, so do opinions and information in our social circles. Since ‘birds of a feather flock together,’ these social circles consist mostly of like-minded individuals, making it harder for dissenting views to permeate our discourse, and creating an echo of our own information and views. This is the echo chamber phenomenon that has been drawing growing concern in a post-election US, a post-Brexit UK, and pretty much everywhere in our increasingly connected world.

Echo chambers have always been around, but as our information gathering, social networking, and conversations shift online, they seem to be getting worse. Who should we blame – ourselves, or filtering algorithms beyond our control? Bakshy and colleagues at Facebook research labs find that individuals’ own choices are more to blame for their echo chambers than Facebook’s newsfeed ranking algorithm (Bakshy et al. 2015). Since our past choices and clicks feed into personalisation algorithms (El-Bermawy 2016), the two mechanisms are mutually reinforcing, and will create deeper echo chambers in the long run.

In recent research, we study what types of users are most likely to get caught in a content echo chamber, and the role of popularity information provision in facilitating them (Bar-Gill and Gandal 2017). This online experiment examines users’ content exploration and choice patterns in a simplified search environment where users separately control two search dimensions: topic and popularity.

In this setting, susceptibility to echo chambers is proxied by:

- conducting relatively little exploration; and

- relying on popularity in content choice.

Intuitively, little exploration along with reliance on the crowd’s tastes will lead, over time, to reduced exposure to diverse content, thereby facilitating the creation of an echo chamber.

The experimental setting

Our search environment – TED-it1 – allows users to browse the collection of TED2 talks (roughly 1,600 short videos) using two buttons: “Category” and “Popularity”.3 Users search to find a talk they would like to watch.4

Figure 1 The TED-it new search screen

Image: VoxEU

The Category button enables topical search, and opens a dropdown list of 15 categories. Choosing one of these produces a list of talks in the chosen category, in random order. Clicking the Popularity button sorts the displayed search results by their number of views on YouTube, from most to least popular. When Popularity is clicked before a category has been chosen, the button produces a sorted list of all talks. Users may click each of the buttons as many times as they like, creating a search sequence. Figure 2 shows a screenshot with search results appearing after a user clicked on Category and chose the Entertainment category.

Figure 2 Search results on TED-it

Image: VoxEU

Users’ personal and social characteristics, previous experience with TED content, and opinion leadership were assessed using a series of short self-report questionnaires. We recruited 1,846 experiment participants via Amazon Mechanical Turk. See our paper (Bar-Gill and Gandal 2017) for descriptive statistics on these participants and further details on the questionnaires used.

Content exploration and choice on TED-it

The average TED-it user performed 0.65 Popularity clicks and 1.05 Category clicks. Fully 35% of users clicked only on Popularity, thereby sorting all talks, and choosing from this list.

Much like other distributions of media consumption, the distribution of views on TED-it appears long-tailed. Our users watched 532 different talks. The number of views for the top five talks is 123, 116, 69, 69, and 63, respectively, whereas talks ranked between 107 and 532 had one or two views each (see Figure 3a).

While TED-it presents information on talks’ YouTube view-counts, and allows for sorting based on these, the rank order of views on TED-it does not mirror that on YouTube. This is evident from panel (b) of Figure 3, which shows YouTube view-counts for talks ordered by their TED-it views.

Figure 3 Distribution of view counts for top 100 talks viewed on TED-it

(a) Distribution of view numbers on TED-it

Image: VoxEU

(b) Distribution of view numbers on Youtube

Image: VoxEU

How does the view-count distribution change with users’ exploration patterns? There are three possible end-states of an exploration process:

(a) Unsorted category – an unsorted list, within category
(b) Sorted category – a popularity-sorted list, within category
(c) All sorted – a sorted list of all talks (no category chosen)

Intuitively, popularity reliance increases monotonically from (a) to (c). Validating our notion of popularity reliance, the mean YouTube view count is 521,007; 2,909,773; and 5,032,500 in end-states (a), (b), and (c) respectively. The correlation between the TED-it (our experiment) and YouTube view counts increases from ρ = 0.12 for (a)-users to ρ = 0.69 for (c)-users.

Lower popularity reliance leads users to content that is less known by their peers, and is more suited to their personal interests. We argue that users who consistently search by their topics of interest and rely less on the crowd’s previous choices are less likely to suffer from echo chambers. Susceptibility to echo chambers in our setting is represented by a low utilisation of category search, along with a strong reliance on popularity sorting.

Echo chamber risk factors: Sociability, familiarity with content, ageSociability

We conjecture that highly social individuals will tend to ‘follow the herd’ and suffer a risk of echo chambers, since they identify strongly with their peers. Indeed, we find that highly social individuals show a weaker preference for topical exploration, and rely more heavily on popularity sorting, compared to individuals reporting lower sociability. We further find that highly social individuals perform fewer search clicks and are less likely to choose from unsorted category-specific results (see Figure 4).

Figure 4 Sociability and topic based exploration, males vs. females

(a) Share of first clicks on Category

Image: VoxEU

(b) Share of talks chosen from unsorted Category results

Image: VoxEU

Familiarity with content

Previous experience with a content space is expected to decrease the tendency to explore in that space, and thus content familiarity is an echo chambers risk factor. The data corroborates this hypothesis for male users. Figure 5 shows that users who have previously watched TED talks are more likely to begin their exploration by sorting all talks, and are less likely to choose a talk from an unsorted list. This effect is statistically significant only for males.

Figure 5 Experience with content and topic-based exploration, males vs. females

(a) Share of first clicks on Category

Image: VoxEU

(b) Share of talks chosen from unsorted Category results

Image: VoxEU

Age

Examining the relationship of various user demographics with the observed exploration patterns, we find that younger individuals are less likely to conduct a topical search and rely mostly on popularity considerations. Younger users are thus more likely to get caught in echo chambers than older users.

Opinion leaders may alleviate echo chambers

Opinion leaders seek to influence others in their social circles. Since already-popular content decreases opinion leaders’ capacity to act as thought leaders, we expect these individuals to exhibit lower popularity reliance, and search based on their topical interests, to create new avenues for influence.

In line with this, opinion leadership is found to affect exploration patterns, but only for men in our sample. Specifically, male opinion leaders exhibit a stronger preference for exploration by topic of interest and invest more effort in search, performing more search clicks than non-opinion leaders. Figure 6 depicts male opinion leaders’ higher tendency to explore by topic and to choose from an unsorted category-specific list (though only the former is statistically significant).

These results suggest that opinion leaders tend to introduce new information and may thus alleviate echo chambers in their social circles.

Figure 6 Opinion leadership and topic-based exploration, males vs. females

(a) Share of first clicks on Category

Image: VoxEU

(b) Share of talks chosen from unsorted Category results

Image: VoxEU

The role of popularity information

Popularity information, such as Facebook like-counts and YouTube view-counts, are baked into our online environments. How is this affecting our tendency to explore unfamiliar territories? On the one hand, popularity information can substitute for sorting, by providing a quality guarantee for content that is not ‘topping the charts’ (i.e. generating thousands, but not millions, of views). On the other hand, it can create increased awareness of popularity considerations and complement them, leading to a stronger reliance on the crowd’s tastes. Whether providing popularity information is a substitute or a complement to sorting is thus an empirical question, which we answer by randomly assigning users to one of two conditions, where popularity information is shown or blocked in category-based searches.

Interestingly, only male opinion leaders are affected by the provision of popularity information, complementing this information with increased sorting, as seen in Figure 7. The strong topic preference observed for these users is therefore weakened in the presence of information on the crowd’s choices.

Figure 7. Effect of popularity information provision on tendency to choose from unsorted, category-specific, search results

Image: VoxEU

Notes: Share of talks chosen from unsorted Category with and without display of popularity information for opinion leaders vs. non-leaders.

Concluding note

Should we suppress, or at least reduce the visibility of, popularity information in our online environments to increase the diversity of the content consumed? To the extent that content diversity is a desired end, the answer is yes. Furthermore, in all our analyses, there were no statistically significant correlations between users’ exploration characteristics and proxies for enjoyment, such as viewing talks past the mandated five minutes or watching an extra talk. This suggests that in the realm of TED talks, popular content is not inherently superior to less popular content. Therefore, designing online environments that encourage topic-based exploration (e.g. with increased visibility of non-hit content, and reduced visibility of view counts) may ameliorate content echo chambers, with little impact on user satisfaction.

References

Bakshy, E, S Messing and L A Adamic (2015) “Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook”, Science, 348(6239): 1130-1132.

Bar-Gill, S and N Gandal (2017) “Content exploration, choice and echo chambers: An experiment”, CEPR, Discussion paper DP11909.

El-Bermawy, M (2016) “Your filter bubble is destroying democracy”, Wired, 18 November.

Endnotes

[2] TED is a non-profit organisation devoted to spreading ideas, usually in the form of short talks (18 minutes or less). TED stands for technology, entertainment and design, though TED talks today may cover any topic (more at https://www.ted.com/about/).

[3] The location of the buttons is randomised, such that Popularity appears on the right only for 50% of the users. This is to rule out possible location effects.

[4] Users had to watch at least five minutes of the lecture in order to receive payment for participation. This requirement has been shown to motivate non-trivial search activity in an early stage pilot.

Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Related topics:
Industries in DepthFourth Industrial Revolution
Share:
World Economic Forum logo
Global Agenda

The Agenda Weekly

A weekly update of the most important issues driving the global agenda

Subscribe today

You can unsubscribe at any time using the link in our emails. For more details, review our privacy policy.

Industry government collaboration on agritech can empower global agriculture

Abhay Pareek and Drishti Kumar

April 23, 2024

1:44

About Us

Events

Media

Partners & Members

  • Join Us

Language Editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

© 2024 World Economic Forum