Emerging Technologies

How ‘mental rehearsal’ prepares us for action

A man watches seasonal autumn storm clouds gather over the sea ahead of steady rain off the coast of Sydney March 8, 2007. Many parts of Australia are in the grip of the worst drought in 100 years, forcing authorities to impose water restrictions exacerbated when rainfall misses catchment areas. REUTERS/Will Burgess      (AUSTRALIA)

Neuroscientist have found that imagining a situation before it happens can improve our chances of success. Image: REUTERS/Will Burgess

Nathan Collins
Share:
Our Impact
What's the World Economic Forum doing to accelerate action on Emerging Technologies?
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Neuroscience is affecting economies, industries and global issues
A hand holding a looking glass by a lake
Crowdsource Innovation
Get involved with our crowdsourced digital platform to deliver impact at scale
Stay up to date:

Neuroscience

Neuroscientists have discovered how the brain learns physical tasks, even in the absence of real-world movement.

It could hinge on getting the mind to the right starting place and to be ready to perfectly execute everything that follows with a process called “mental rehearsal.”

Psychologists and athletes alike know that it works: picturing ourselves going through routines, whether it’s figure skating or something more mundane, improves our chances of success.

“Mental rehearsal is tantalizing, but difficult to study,” says Saurabh Vyas, a graduate student in bioengineering at Stanford University and the lead author of a new paper that appears in Neuron. That’s because there’s no easy way to peer into a person’s brain as he imagines himself racing to a win or practicing a performance.

“This is where we thought brain-machine interfaces could be that lens, because they give you the ability to see what the brain is doing even when they’re not actually moving,” he says.

Although there are some important caveats, the results could point the way toward a deeper understanding of what mental rehearsal is. Further, researchers believe the findings could lead to a future where brain-machine interfaces, usually thought of as prosthetics for people with paralysis, are also tools for understanding the brain, says senior author Krishna Shenoy, a professor in the School of Engineering at Stanford and a member of Stanford Bio-X and the Stanford Neurosciences Institute.

From thought to action

The idea for the study came while thinking about how people learn to use brain-machine interfaces to perform a task, Vyas says. In a typical setup, a person—or, very often, a monkey—has to learn to move a cursor around a computer screen using only patterns of activity in the brain, not hand or other movements. That got Vyas wondering whether what people (or monkeys) learned using brain-machine interfaces might somehow transfer, in a way similar to mental rehearsal, to physical movements.

“He’s just sitting there thinking, and as he’s thinking he’s getting better and better” at moving the cursor, Vyas says, referring to one of the monkeys he studied. “The natural question becomes: What happens if you switch to another context, where now he actually has to generate muscle activity? Do you see the effects of that learning in that new context?”

The short answer is “yes,” mental learning does transfer to physical performance. Vyas initially taught two monkeys outfitted with brain-machine interfaces to move a cursor from one place to another on a computer screen using only their minds, then introduced a complication, called a visuomotor rotation: what mental signals they previously used to move a cursor up would now move it at an angle, say 45 degrees clockwise.

The monkeys easily adapted, and that adaptation carried over when they repeated the same task using their hands, rather than the brain-machine interface, to control the cursor directly. Now, if the monkeys wanted to move the cursor up, they moved their hands 45 degrees clockwise.

This suggested that the monkeys were doing something like mental rehearsal, Vyas says—what they had learned to do in their minds, they could then do with their hands.

Some additional experiments and an analysis of recorded neural activity suggest the reason why: rehearsing the task with a brain-machine interface got patterns of activity in the monkeys’ brains into just the right spot, so they could carry out the same rotation task with their hands, even though they had never done so before.

‘A new tool’

“There are key differences between our paradigm and true mental rehearsal,” Vyas says, and there are reasons to be cautious about interpreting the results too broadly.

For one thing, you can’t just ask a monkey to imagine completing a physical activity, as you could with a person. For another, mentally rehearsing a task is not the same as using a brain-machine interface to do it. In the latter case, people get feedback on how they’re doing, something they can only imagine in mental rehearsal.

“We can’t prove the connection beyond a shadow of a doubt,” Shenoy says, but “this is a major step in understanding what mental rehearsal may well be in all of us.”

Have you read?

The next steps, he and Vyas say, are to figure out how mental rehearsal relates to practice with a brain-machine interface—and how mental preparation, the key ingredient in transferring that practice to physical movements, relates to movement.

Meanwhile, Shenoy says, the results demonstrate the potential of an entirely new tool for studying the mind. “It’s like building a new tool and using it for something,” Shenoy says. “We used a brain-machine interface to probe and advance basic science, and that’s just super exciting.”

Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Related topics:
Emerging TechnologiesEducation and Skills
Share:
World Economic Forum logo
Global Agenda

The Agenda Weekly

A weekly update of the most important issues driving the global agenda

Subscribe today

You can unsubscribe at any time using the link in our emails. For more details, review our privacy policy.

Equitable healthcare is the industry's north star. Here's how AI can get us there

Vincenzo Ventricelli

April 25, 2024

About Us

Events

Media

Partners & Members

  • Join Us

Language Editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

© 2024 World Economic Forum