Inequality

Correlation and causality in economics: Can we prove it?

Silhouetted workers walk in front of office towers in the Canary Wharf financial district in London February 16, 2011. New Bank of England forecasts opened the door on Wednesday for interest rates to rise slowly in Britain but Governor Mervyn King warned against jumping to conclusions about when the central bank would pull the trigger.

These are three options to tease out causality, but we shouldn't look for one to 'rule over the others', argues Nick Bunker. Image: REUTERS/Luke MacGregor

Nick Bunker
Policy Analyst, Washington Center for Equitable Growth
Share:
The Big Picture
Explore and monitor how Inequality is affecting economies, industries and global issues
A hand holding a looking glass by a lake
Crowdsource Innovation
Get involved with our crowdsourced digital platform to deliver impact at scale
Stay up to date:

Inequality

In the most recent interview in our “Equitable Growth in Conversation” series, our own Ben Zipperer talks with David Card of the University of California, Berkeley and Alan Krueger of Princeton University. The interview covers a number of areas, but it centers on Card and Krueger’s role in advancing empirical methods that help show causality. As you’ve probably heard a few hundred times in your life, correlation doesn’t imply causation. But the two economists sparked thinking about how other researchers could show the actual causal impact of a new policy or a shock to the economy—and this thinking is now a key part of the profession.

 Years of education and season of birth
Image: Angriest and Krueger 1991

When it comes to showing causation in a hard science like physics or chemistry, the path forward is relatively well tread: Set up an experiment that controls for all the factors except the one you want to understand the impact of. But that’s not exactly possible when it comes to a social science like economics. You might want to understand what happens to economic growth when you implement a new policy, but it’s really hard to sort out the impact of everything else going on in the economy.

But researchers, like Card and Krueger, have figured out ways to tease out causality—one of which is a natural experiment. Think of Card and Krueger’s famous study on the minimum wage, for example. They used the fact that the minimum wage was going up in New Jersey but not Pennsylvania, and then looked at what happened to employment at fast food restaurants along the border in both states. The intent of raising the minimum wage wasn’t to cause an experiment, but the two economists used it as such.

Another way to get at causal effect is to use an instrumental variable. Let’s say you’re trying to understand how the amount of schooling a person has affects the wages he or she will earn. You’ll run into the problem that there are factors that affect how long a person will attend school while also affecting how much they might earn, such as innate talent. So how would you tease out just the effect of an exact year of school? You could find something that’s strongly correlated with the amount of time spent in school but not correlated at all with the other factors—say, talent—that might also affect wages, and then see how that affects how much a person earns.

A third option is to set up something very close to a real-world experiment, in a technique known as a randomized controlled trial, or RCT. A good example is the well-known Oregon Health Insurance Experiment. In 2008, the state of Oregon had enough money to expand its Medicaid plan but not enough money to expand it to all the people who wanted it. So the state ended up using a lottery to decide who got Medicaid and who didn’t. As the lottery was random, researchers could compare very similar people with and without health insurance knowing that difference was determined by nothing but chance. That certainty about randomness helps show the causal impact of health insurance.

But as Card says in the interview, all these methods have “some strengths and some weaknesses.” Natural experiments, for example, are great because they actually happen in the real world, but it’s not always clear how random they are. It’s tough to know how random the changes to policy are. At the same time, a randomized controlled trial is great for knowing the effect of the change in the trial to a great degree of confidence, but it’s hard to know how applicable those results are to circumstances outside those that happened during the trial. So while it might be nice to be able to point to one technique to rule over all others, giving such power to one technique probably isn’t for the best.

Don't miss any update on this topic

Create a free account and access your personalized content collection with our latest publications and analyses.

Sign up for free

License and Republishing

World Economic Forum articles may be republished in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International Public License, and in accordance with our Terms of Use.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author alone and not the World Economic Forum.

Related topics:
InequalityEconomic Progress
Share:
World Economic Forum logo
Global Agenda

The Agenda Weekly

A weekly update of the most important issues driving the global agenda

Subscribe today

You can unsubscribe at any time using the link in our emails. For more details, review our privacy policy.

This is why the number of refugees could double in the next decade, according to the head of UNHCR

Liam Coleman

March 7, 2024

About Us

Events

Media

Partners & Members

  • Join Us

Language Editions

Privacy Policy & Terms of Service

© 2024 World Economic Forum